フィンランドで実施されたベーシックインカムに関する社会実験

An experiment to inform universal basic income

By Tera Allas, Jukka Maksimainen, James Manyika, and Navjot Singh  September 15, 2020 / Article / McKinsey & Company

わたしのnoteにおいては、最新の科学・経済・社会等の問題に関して、英語の記事を引用し、その英文が読み易いように加工し、「英語の勉強ツール」と「最新情報収集ツール」としてご利用頂くことをmain missionとさせて頂きます。勿論、私論を書かせて頂くこともしばしです。

ベーシックインカムの社会実験の結果が紹介されているが、無作為化コントロール実験の実施においては、様々な社会的要因が絡むため、明確な結論を導くことはなかなかハードルが高い。しかし、本実験で明らかにされたこともあり、雇用や家計への影響を除外しても、以下の様な様々なpositiveな効果が認められ、ベーシックインカムの社会実験を実施する際には、複合的な効果を多面的に評価することが求められる。
● 
better well-being on multiple dimensions
● life satisfaction
● better health and lower levels of stress, depression, sadness, and loneliness—all major determinants of happiness
● more confidence in their cognitive skills, assessing their ability to remember, learn, and concentrate at higher levels
● perceive their financial situation as more secure and manageable, even though their incomes were no higher than those of people in the control group
● expressed higher levels of trust in their own future, their fellow citizens, and public institutions
● liberating effect on many recipients
● Better feelings of health, happiness, cognitive abilities, and financial security seem to have instilled a sense of confidence that encouraged the recipients to branch out and to seek more expansive opportunities: unpaid work, training, or employment.

As income inequality and economic upheaval take center stage, / is a guaranteed minimum income worth considering? Results from a two-year experiment in Finland offer clues.

Readers who are familiar with [the inequality debate] or [the fears that robots will take all of our jobs] will almost certainly have heard about the idea of a universal basic income. As typically conceived, basic-income programs are meant to provide a financial safety net, with no obligations and without the bureaucracy and associated administrative costs of means-tested benefits. Trends in globalization and automation, as well as the rapid rise in the cost of necessities, such as housing, had already started to put pressure on social contracts in many countries before the pandemic. Now, with COVID-19 creating additional economic risks—especially for already-vulnerable groups—questions about / how best to support people living on low incomes / are bound to become even more important.

The body of quantitative evidence for or against a universal basic income (UBI) is still slim. The context and design of the first wave of policies, from 1960 to 1980 and primarily in North America, make the results hard to generalize. In the 2000s, a new wave of experiments—some funded by charities rather than governments—has sprung up. Municipalities (地方自治体/mjuːnìsəpǽləti) in the Netherlands, Barcelona in Spain, the US city of Stockton, in California, the Brazilian city of Maricá, and the province of Gyeonggi in South Korea are among the places experimenting with a basic income. 

Research-based policy making

Randomized control trials (RCTs) are difficult to execute in public policy. To draw robust conclusions, researchers must follow best practices to ensure that study groups are large enough and representative of each demographic group of interest. The control environment should be free of external disturbances that could influence the study’s outcomes. To avoid putting a study’s power and credibility at risk, researchers must resist pressures to rush the results.

Finland’s pilot program suffered from all of these issues. It was imperfect in many respects. The number of people receiving the basic income instead of normal unemployment benefits was only 2,000. New unemployment policies were implemented halfway through the experiment. Despite the initial aspirations of the research team, it could test only a single version of the policy rather than many variants. There was even initial ambiguity about which effects the study should track, before ultimately settling on correlations with employment, financial well-being, health, mental well-being, and trust.

Some of the results—for instance, those on employment—were not conclusive, and many aspects of the study and findings were criticized and disputed. However, in the end, even with these flaws, the completed experiment and the associated research have shed important light on the complex considerations and implications of a basic income.

However, to date Finland is the only country that has managed to complete a nationwide randomized control trial of a basic-income program. The research methods used were particularly diverse and included literature reviews, microsimulations, surveys, data linking, in-depth interviews, and media analysis. In this article, we highlight the insights that we found most interesting. More research is needed in this multifaceted and complex area, not least because of the many unanswered questions on how a universal basic income could be funded—and how it would interact with other sources of government assistance. It is also worth noting that the methods and conclusions of the Finnish study were not undisputed.

In Finland’s two-year study, a treatment group of 2,000 randomly picked, initially unemployed people received a guaranteed, unconditional, and automatic cash payment of a modest €560 (560X120円=67200円) per month instead of a basic unemployment allowance in similar amounts. Even with a housing allowance, which basic-income recipients were eligible for, this level of support was significantly below the incomes of most Finnish households. All other unemployed people, who continued to receive standard benefits, formed the control group.

The final results from Finland’s experiment are now in, and the findings are intriguing: the basic income in Finland led to a small increase in employment, significantly boosted multiple measures of the recipients’ well-being, and reinforced positive individual and societal feedback loops.

A small increase in employment

In the design of the Finnish experiment, the main research question, agreed to by parliament in the enabling legislation, was the impact of a basic income on employment. Many policy makers assume that an entirely unconditional guaranteed income would reduce incentives to work. After all, the argument goes, why bother with a job if you can have a decent life without one (basic income)? This assumption has led many countries to deploy active labor-market policies that require people on unemployment benefits to prove their eligibility continually and, often, to participate in some kind of training or to accept jobs offered to them.

Interestingly, the final results of Finland’s program, released this spring, found that a basic income actually had a positive impact on employment. People on the basic income were more likely to be employed than those in the control group, and the differences were statistically significant, albeit small. Concurrent changes in other unemployment policies make it difficult to ascertain, from this study, whether the basic income, the other changes, or both were responsible for the higher employment levels. However, something about the modest level of the basic income and the lack of conditions attached to receiving it seems to have motivated recipients to seek and accept work they otherwise might not have. 

A critical lesson of the Finnish experiment is the complexity of implementing a basic income. Policy makers need to decide how it should interact with a large number of other policies, such as child benefits, housing benefits, pensions, health insurance, and taxation; for example, in the Finnish experiment, basic-income recipients were eligible for housing allowances but not for basic social-assistance payments. Unless such linkages are streamlined, they could detract (損なう、減じる、落とす/ditrǽkt) from a basic-income system’s potentially considerable savings in administrative costs.

Such interactions emphasize the importance of running further experiments and tracking outcomes across a wide range of well-being factors, including not only employment and financial security but also health and happiness. Of course, the effects will vary from one group to another.

A huge boost to well-being

However you read the findings on employment, other effects were clear: people on the basic income reported significantly better well-being on multiple dimensions. Average life satisfaction among the treatment group was 7.3 out of 10, compared with 6.8 in the control group—a very large increase. To experience a similar lift in life satisfaction, we estimate that a person’s income would need to go up by as much as €800 (800X120円=96000円) to €2,500 (2500X120円=300000円) per month—60 to 170 percent of the average per-capita household income in the European Union. Indeed, the difference was big enough to erase the gap in life satisfaction between unemployed and employed people.

These significant positive findings on well-being are no mystery: the basic income seems to have improved all the major components of life satisfaction. People receiving the basic income reported better health and lower levels of stress, depression, sadness, and loneliness—all major determinants of happiness—than people in the control group. Recipients of the basic income also demonstrated more confidence in their cognitive skills, assessing their ability to remember, learn, and concentrate at higher levels than the control group did. And the basic income enabled people to perceive their financial situation as more secure and manageable, even though their incomes were no higher than those of people in the control group. Finally, basic-income recipients expressed higher levels of trust in their own future, their fellow citizens, and public institutions.

Positive feedback loops

The basic income also appears to have had an effect on the dynamic cause-and-effect loops that trap some people in deprivation while others thrive on multiple dimensions. In other words, a relatively small positive intervention seems to have generated multiple mutually reinforcing positive effects. These dynamics could completely change the typical calculus of cost–benefit analyses. The Finnish study finds that the basic income unlocked at least two virtuous cycles: one that operates at the level of individuals (and their families) and another at the level of society.

At the individual level, a monthly, guaranteed, and entirely unconditional cash sum had a liberating effect on many recipients. Better feelings of health, happiness, cognitive abilities, and financial security seem to have instilled a sense of confidence that encouraged the recipients to branch out and to seek more expansive opportunities: unpaid work, training, or employment. These activities, in turn, fueled more positive feelings. The recipients’ trust in their own abilities and their positive outlook seem to have acted as self-fulfilling prophecies (予言/prɑ́fəsi). In contrast, research has found that people experiencing scarcity and uncertainty tend to suffer from reduced bandwidth (仕事に関わる余力/bǽndwìdθ), shortened time horizons, and feelings of inadequacy or helplessness. 

At a societal level, Finland’s basic-income experiment promoted another interesting virtuous cycle, around trust. Trust in others and institutions is a fundamental building block of well-functioning societies. When researchers look for determinants of both well-being and economic prosperity, trust regularly crops up (現れる・出現する・起こる) as a key variable. Indeed, Finland is a case in point: it ranks at the top of global measures of happiness and also boasts the second-highest rating on trust in other people, after Norway.

When people trust institutions, such as the police, the judiciary, and public services, their trust in others also tends to increase, so it isn’t trivial that Finland’s basic-income program improved that level of trust. At the end of the two years, basic-income recipients registered elevated levels of trust in other people and institutions, such as Finland’s politicians, political parties, parliament, judiciary, and social-security system. One explanation could be that the basic-income experiment did not involve bureaucracy, another that the recipients felt society—or “the system”—was not neglecting people who had fallen on hard times.

As with any policy analysis, the results of this experiment remain subject to debate and can’t necessarily be generalized. As a result, the experiment does offer an object lesson (教訓) in the complexity of designing and implementing a randomized control trial of basic income. Nevertheless, more research on basic income is required. We can hope that Finland’s example will inform and inspire others as they set up their own experiments.

いいなと思ったら応援しよう!